Browsing by Author "McShea, Daniel W"
Results Per Page
Sort Options
Item Open Access A quantitative formulation of biology's first law.(Evolution; international journal of organic evolution, 2019-06) McShea, Daniel W; Wang, Steve C; Brandon, Robert NThe zero-force evolutionary law (ZFEL) states that in evolutionary systems, in the absence of forces or constraints, diversity and complexity tend to increase. The reason is that diversity and complexity are both variance measures, and variances tend to increase spontaneously as random events accumulate. Here, we use random-walk models to quantify the ZFEL expectation, producing equations that give the probabilities of diversity or complexity increasing as a function of time, and that give the expected magnitude of the increase. We produce two sets of equations, one for the case in which variation occurs in discrete steps, the other for the case in which variation is continuous. The equations provide a way to decompose actual trajectories of diversity or complexity into two components, the portion due to the ZFEL and a remainder due to selection and constraint. Application of the equations is demonstrated using real and hypothetical data.Item Open Access Culture From Infrahumans to Humans: Essays in the Philosophy of Biology(2007-05-07T19:07:23Z) Ramsey, Grant AaronIt has become increasingly common to explain the behavior of animals—from sperm whales to songbirds—in terms of culture. But what is animal culture, what is its relationship to other biological concepts and to human culture, and what impact does culture have on a species’ evolution and ecology? My dissertation is an attempt to answer these questions. After an introductory chapter, the dissertation begins (Chapter 2) with a proposal for a novel concept of culture and a critique of the existing ways in which culture has been characterized. These characterizations include views from cultural anthropology as well as attempts to apply the concept of culture to animals. The existing concepts are problematic in a number of ways, such as a priori excluding infrahumans from being candidates for possessing culture, or mistaking what culture is for its measure. In this chapter I offer a way to understand culture that avoids these and other problems. With a concept of culture in hand, the next chapter of my dissertation (Chapter 3) examines and criticizes one key way of understanding the concept of culture, meme theory. In Chapter 4 I turn to the question of how cultural systems can arise in nature, how they can be adaptive, and how the evolution and ecology of species is impacted by the possession of a cultural system. In order to answer these questions I introduce a general constraint on cultural systems—what I am calling the Fundamental Constraint—that has to be satisfied in order for cultural systems to be adaptive. In the final chapter I develop a concept of innovation and draw out the conceptual and empirical implications of this concept.Item Open Access Evolution on Arbitrary Fitness Landscapes when Mutation is Weak(2012) McCandlish, David MartinEvolutionary dynamics can be notoriously complex and difficult to analyze. In this dissertation I describe a population genetic regime where the dynamics are simple enough to allow a relatively complete and elegant treatment. Consider a haploid, asexual population, where each possible genotype has been assigned a fitness. When mutations enter a population sufficiently rarely, we can model the evolution of this population as a Markov chain where the population jumps from one genotype to another at the birth of each new mutant destined for fixation. Furthermore, if the mutation rates are assigned in such a manner that the Markov chain is reversible when all genotypes are assigned the same fitness, then it is still reversible when genotypes are assigned differing fitnesses.
The key insight is that this Markov chain can be analyzed using the spectral theory of finite-state, reversible Markov chains. I describe the spectral decomposition of the transition matrix and use it to build a general framework with which I address a variety of both classical and novel topics. These topics include a method for creating low-dimensional visualizations of fitness landscapes; a measure of how easy it is for the evolutionary process to `find' a specific genotype or phenotype; the index of dispersion of the molecular clock and its generalizations; a definition for the neighborhood of a genotype based on evolutionary dynamics; and the expected fitness and number of substitutions that have occurred given that a population has been evolving on the fitness landscape for a given period of time. I apply these various analyses to both a simple one-codon fitness landscape and to a large neutral network derived from computational RNA secondary structure predictions.
Item Open Access Evolutionary trends and goal directedness.(Synthese, 2023-01) McShea, Daniel WThe conventional wisdom declares that evolution is not goal directed, that teleological considerations play no part in our understanding of evolutionary trends. Here I argue that, to the contrary, under a current view of teleology, field theory, most evolutionary trends would have to be considered goal directed to some degree. Further, this view is consistent with a modern scientific outlook, and more particularly with evolutionary theory today. Field theory argues that goal directedness is produced by higher-level fields that direct entities contained within them to behave persistently and plastically, that is, returning them to a goal-directed trajectory following perturbations (persistence) and directing them to a goal-directed trajectory from a large range of alternative starting points (plasticity). The behavior of a bacterium climbing a chemical food gradient is persistent and plastic, with guidance provided by the external "food field," the chemical gradient. Likewise, an evolutionary trend that is produced by natural selection is a lineage behaving persistently and plastically under the direction of its local ecology, an "ecological field." Trends directed by selection-generated boundaries, thermodynamic gradients, and certain internal constraints, would also count as goal directed. In other words, most of the causes of evolutionary trends that have been proposed imply goal directedness. However, under field theory, not all trends are goal directed. Examples are discussed. Importantly, nothing in this view suggests that evolution is guided by intentionality, at least none at the level of animal intentionality. Finally, possible implications for our thinking about evolutionary directionality in the history of life are discussed.Item Open Access Evolutionary Trends in the Individuation and Polymorphism of Colonial Marine Invertebrates(2007-05-10T16:02:15Z) Venit, Edward PeterAll life is organized hierarchically. Lower levels, such as cells and zooids, are nested within higher levels, such as multicellular organisms and colonial animals. The process by which a higher-level unit forms from the coalescence of lower-level units is known as “individuation”. Individuation is defined by the strength of functional interdependencies among constituent lower-level units. Interdependency results from division of labor, which is evidenced in colonial metazoans as zooid polymorphism. As lower-level units specialize for certain tasks, they become increasing dependant on the rest of the collective to perform other tasks. In this way, the evolution of division of labor drives the process of individuation. This study explores several ways in which polymorphism evolves in colonial marine invertebrates such as cnidarians, bryozoans, and urochordates. A previous study on the effect of environmental stability on polymorphism is revisted and reinterpreted. A method for quantifying colonial-level individuation by measuring the spatial arrangement of polymorphic zooids is proposed and demonstrated. Most significantly, a comparison across all colonial marine invertebrate taxa reveals that polymorphism only appears in those colonial taxa with moderately to strongly compartmentalized zooids. Weakly compartmentalized and fully compartmentalized taxa are universally monomorphic. This pattern is seen across all colonial marine invertebrate taxa and is interpreted as a “rule” governing the evolution of higher-level individuation in the major taxa of colonial marine invertebrates. The existence of one rule suggests that there may be others, including rules that transcend levels of biological hierarchy. The identification of such rules would strongly suggest that new levels in the hierarchy of life evolve by a universal pattern that is independent of the type of organism involved.Item Open Access Four reasons for scepticism about a human major transition in social individuality.(Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 2023-03) McShea, Daniel WThe 'major transitions in evolution' are mainly about the rise of hierarchy, new individuals arising at ever higher levels of nestedness, in particular the eukaryotic cell arising from prokaryotes, multicellular individuals from solitary protists and individuated societies from multicellular individuals. Some lists include human societies as a major transition, but based on a comparison with the non-human transitions, there are reasons for scepticism. (i) The foundation of the major transitions is hierarchy, but the cross-cutting interactions in human societies undermine hierarchical structure. (ii) Natural selection operates in three modes-stability, growth and reproductive success-and only the third produces the complex adaptations seen in fully individuated higher levels. But human societies probably evolve mainly in the stability and growth modes. (iii) Highly individuated entities are marked by division of labour and commitment to morphological differentiation, but in humans differentiation is mostly behavioural and mostly reversible. (iv) As higher-level individuals arise, selection drains complexity, drains parts, from lower-level individuals. But there is little evidence of a drain in humans. In sum, a comparison with the other transitions gives reasons to doubt that human social individuation has proceeded very far, or if it has, to doubt that it is a transition of the same sort. This article is part of the theme issue 'Human socio-cultural evolution in light of evolutionary transitions'.Item Open Access Freedom and purpose in biology.(Studies in history and philosophy of biological and biomedical sciences, 2016-08) McShea, Daniel WAll seemingly teleological systems share a common hierarchical structure. They consist of a small entity moving or changing within a larger field that directs it from above (what I call "upper direction"). This is true for organisms seeking some external resource, for the organized behavior of cells and other parts in organismal development, and for lineages evolving by natural selection. In all cases, the lower-level entity is partly "free," tending to wander under the influence of purely local forces, and partly directed by a larger enveloping field. The persistent and plastic behavior that characterizes goal-directedness arises, I argue, at intermediate levels of freedom and upper direction, when the two are in a delicate balance. I tentatively extend the argument to human teleology (wants, purposes).Item Open Access Hierarchical complexity and the size limits of life.(Proceedings. Biological sciences, 2017-06) Heim, Noel A; Payne, Jonathan L; Finnegan, Seth; Knope, Matthew L; Kowalewski, Michał; Lyons, S Kathleen; McShea, Daniel W; Novack-Gottshall, Philip M; Smith, Felisa A; Wang, Steve COver the past 3.8 billion years, the maximum size of life has increased by approximately 18 orders of magnitude. Much of this increase is associated with two major evolutionary innovations: the evolution of eukaryotes from prokaryotic cells approximately 1.9 billion years ago (Ga), and multicellular life diversifying from unicellular ancestors approximately 0.6 Ga. However, the quantitative relationship between organismal size and structural complexity remains poorly documented. We assessed this relationship using a comprehensive dataset that includes organismal size and level of biological complexity for 11 172 extant genera. We find that the distributions of sizes within complexity levels are unimodal, whereas the aggregate distribution is multimodal. Moreover, both the mean size and the range of size occupied increases with each additional level of complexity. Increases in size range are non-symmetric: the maximum organismal size increases more than the minimum. The majority of the observed increase in organismal size over the history of life on the Earth is accounted for by two discrete jumps in complexity rather than evolutionary trends within levels of complexity. Our results provide quantitative support for an evolutionary expansion away from a minimal size constraint and suggest a fundamental rescaling of the constraints on minimal and maximal size as biological complexity increases.Item Open Access Hierarchical Transition Via Individuation, Not Integration: How the Filamentous Fungi Challenge the Standard Model(2012) Crawford, David RobertIn this project I expand the current model of hierarchical transition to include transition by individuation in addition to transition by integration and I apply my model of transition by individuation to the evolution and development of the filamentous fungi. I accomplish this in two parts. In the first section, I defend a general Hierarchy Thesis: A differentiated hierarchical whole can arise not only through the integration of individuated parts but also by the individuation of parts within an integrated whole. I elaborate an expanded model of hierarchical transition and discuss the relevance of part-level selection and part-hood regulation for different modes of transition.
In the second section, I defend a Mycology Thesis: The filamentous fungi have evolved a developmental cellularization process to meet ecological and reproductive demands for coenocytic growth in early development and cellularization in later development. I elaborate the origins and evolution of the filamentous fungi and argue that this history provides cases of hierarchical transition via individuation in both phylogeny and ontogeny.
The project provides an expanded evolutionary-developmental framework for hierarchical transition and a framing narrative for the evolutionary development of filamentous fungi, an evolutionarily significant and ecologically ubiquitous group, and has implications for the study of similar organisms outside Fungi and of hierarchical transition in general.
Item Open Access Imaginative Approaches to Science Education(2016-11-21) Scott, BritainCan an outdoor education curriculum promote environmental awareness and address the needs of children from diverse backgrounds? In what ways can a science curriculum founded in environmental awareness engender inclusivity and help students embrace the idea of life-long learning? Drawing from many different outdoor education curricula and current “best practices,” my research shows the overwhelming need for positive experiences in nature. It is through the use of natural curiosity and wonder that children can develop pro-environmental habits of mind. When a child sees snow melting into a drain on the side of the road they begin to question where that water is going and the impact the chemical residue left on the road may have on its final destination. Environmental stewardship comes from positive experiences in the natural world and a clear understanding of how each and every ecosystem is interconnected. The primary part of my project is a 5th grade curriculum, which aims to encourage exploration and student driven inquiry in order to better understand the world and its systems and to develop a love of learning and the natural world.Item Open Access Logic, passion and the problem of convergence.(Interface focus, 2017-06) McShea, Daniel WOur estimate of the likelihood of convergence on human-style intelligence depends on how we understand our various mental capacities. Here I revive David Hume's theory of motivation and action to argue that the most common understanding of the two conventionally recognized components of intelligence-reason and emotion-is confused. We say things like, 'Reason can overcome emotion', but to make this statement meaningful, we are forced to treat reason as a compound notion, as a forced and unhappy mixture of concepts that are incommensurate. An alternative is to parse intelligence in a different way, into two sets of capacities: (i) non-affective capacities, including logic, calculation and problem-solving; (ii) affective capacities, including wants, preferences and cares, along with the emotions. Thus, the question of convergence becomes two questions, one having to do with affective and one with non-affective capacities. What is the likelihood of convergence of these in non-human lineages, in other ecologies, on other worlds? Given certain assumptions, convergence of the non-affective capacities in thinking species seems likely, I argue, while convergence of the affective capacities seems much less likely.Item Open Access Machine wanting.(Studies in history and philosophy of biological and biomedical sciences, 2013-12) McShea, Daniel WWants, preferences, and cares are physical things or events, not ideas or propositions, and therefore no chain of pure logic can conclude with a want, preference, or care. It follows that no pure-logic machine will ever want, prefer, or care. And its behavior will never be driven in the way that deliberate human behavior is driven, in other words, it will not be motivated or goal directed. Therefore, if we want to simulate human-style interactions with the world, we will need to first understand the physical structure of goal-directed systems. I argue that all such systems share a common nested structure, consisting of a smaller entity that moves within and is driven by a larger field that contains it. In such systems, the smaller contained entity is directed by the field, but also moves to some degree independently of it, allowing the entity to deviate and return, to show the plasticity and persistence that is characteristic of goal direction. If all this is right, then human want-driven behavior probably involves a behavior-generating mechanism that is contained within a neural field of some kind. In principle, for goal directedness generally, the containment can be virtual, raising the possibility that want-driven behavior could be simulated in standard computational systems. But there are also reasons to believe that goal-direction works better when containment is also physical, suggesting that a new kind of hardware may be necessary.Item Open Access The Allometry of Giant Flightless Birds(2007-05-10T14:55:00Z) Dickison, Michael R.Despite our intuition, birds are no smaller than mammals when the constraints of a flying body plan are taken into account. Nevertheless, the largest mammals are ten times the mass of the largest birds. Allometric equations generated for anseriforms and ratites suggest mid-shaft femur circumference is the best measure to use in estimating avian body mass. The small sample size of extant ratites makes mass estimate extrapolation to larger extinct species inaccurate. The division of ratites into cursorial and graviportal groups is supported. Aepyornithids do not show atypical femoral shaft asymmetry. New and more accurate estimates of egg masses, and separate male and female body masses for sexually-dimorphic ratites are generated. Egg mass scaling exponents for individual bird orders differ from that Aves as a whole, probably due to between-taxa effects. Ratite egg mass does not scale with the same exponent as other avian orders, whether kiwi are included or excluded. Total clutch mass in ratites, however, scales similarly to egg mass in other birds, perhaps as a consequence of the extreme variation in ratite clutch size. Kiwi and elephant bird eggs are consistent with the allometric trend for ratites as a whole, taking clutch size into account. Thus kiwi egg mass is probably an adaptation for a precocial life history, not a side effect of their being a dwarfed descendant of a moa-sized ancestor. Relatively small body size in ancestral kiwis is consistent with a trans-oceanic dispersal to New Zealand in the Tertiary, as suggested by recent molecular trees. This implies multiple loss of flight in Tertiary ratite lineages, which is supported by biogeographic, molecular, paleontological, and osteological evidence, but which is not the currently prevailing hypothesis.Item Open Access The Greater Complexity of Drosophila Mutants as Compared to the Wild Type: Part-type, Shape and Color Complexity Over Two Focal Levels(2011) Fleming, LeonoreThe Zero Force Evolutionary Law predicts an increase in complexity at all levels of biological hierarchy unless there are constraints or selective forces opposing that increase. I present the first test of this universal tendency by evaluating the complexity of Drosophila melanogaster mutants, which represent organisms that arise in a context where selective forces are greatly reduced. Complexity gains and losses were measured with respect to part types, shape and color over two independent focal levels. My results show, significantly, that D. melanogaster mutants are more complex than the wild type. I also find that among mutants, those that are weakly constrained are more complex with respect to part types, shape and color. These findings are the first step in testing whether the Zero Force Evolutionary Law is true, and provide the impetus for a larger research program devoted to understanding increases in complexity as the default expectation.
Item Open Access Tool Use by a Predatory Worm(2016-05-10) Taylor, BrianTool use in non-human organisms represents one of the most fiercely contested topics in animal behavior research. Tool use by an animal has been claimed to represent an evolutionarily significant jump in rational thought and ability. Here, I test the hypothesis that Diopatra cuprea are stay-at-home predators who use algae, shells, and sticks as a tool to decorate their tubes in order to serve as an advertisement for attracting prey. Diopatra are a sedentary, tube-dwelling annelid found in great abundance along the intertidal zone. These worms construct and live in long tubes, with the majority of the tube below the sediment surface and a small portion exposed above ground. They then decorate the exposed portion with three different materials: algae, shells, and sticks. To test this hypothesis, I determined what microorganisms took up residence on Diopatra tubes, analyzed the rate at which the worms rebuilt their tubes, and observed the feeding behavior of Diopatra. I found that the same group of microorganisms lived on all three decoration types, but in differing quantities. I also found that if a worm had the exposed portion of its tube destroyed by a disturbance event, it would rebuild the tube almost immediately. I was able to observe Diopatra eating off of their tubes in the laboratory setting and then experimentally determine what their likely food sources are, leading me to the conclusion that the decoration on their tubes serves as a tool for the worms and plays a vital role in their feeding behavior.