Browsing by Author "Tomasello, Michael"
Results Per Page
Sort Options
Item Open Access A comparison of temperament in nonhuman apes and human infants.(Dev Sci, 2011-11) Herrmann, Esther; Hare, Brian; Cissewski, Julia; Tomasello, MichaelThe adaptive behavior of primates, including humans, is often mediated by temperament. Human behavior likely differs from that of other primates in part due to temperament. In the current study we compared the reaction of bonobos, chimpanzees, orangutans, and 2.5-year-old human infants to novel objects and people - as a measure of their shyness-boldness, a key temperamental trait. Human children at the age of 2.5 years avoided novelty of all kinds far more than the other ape species. This response was most similar to that seen in bonobos and least like that of chimpanzees and orangutans. This comparison represents a first step in characterizing the temperamental profiles of species in the hominoid clade, and these findings are consistent with the hypothesis that human temperament has evolved since our lineage diverged from the other apes in ways that likely have broad effects on behavior. These findings also provide new insights into how species differences in ecology may shape differences in temperament.Item Open Access A test of the submentalizing hypothesis: Apes' performance in a false belief task inanimate control.(Commun Integr Biol, 2017) Krupenye, Christopher; Kano, Fumihiro; Hirata, Satoshi; Call, Josep; Tomasello, MichaelMuch debate concerns whether any nonhuman animals share with humans the ability to infer others' mental states, such as desires and beliefs. In a recent eye-tracking false-belief task, we showed that great apes correctly anticipated that a human actor would search for a goal object where he had last seen it, even though the apes themselves knew that it was no longer there. In response, Heyes proposed that apes' looking behavior was guided not by social cognitive mechanisms but rather domain-general cueing effects, and suggested the use of inanimate controls to test this alternative submentalizing hypothesis. In the present study, we implemented the suggested inanimate control of our previous false-belief task. Apes attended well to key events but showed markedly fewer anticipatory looks and no significant tendency to look to the correct location. We thus found no evidence that submentalizing was responsible for apes' anticipatory looks in our false-belief task.Item Open Access Children coordinate in a recurrent social dilemma by taking turns and along dominance asymmetries(Developmental Psychology, 2017-02-01) Grueneisen, Sebastian; Tomasello, Michael© 2016 American Psychological Association.Humans constantly have to coordinate their decisions with others even when their interests are conflicting (e.g., when 2 drivers have to decide who yields at an intersection). So far, however, little is known about the development of these abilities. Here, we present dyads of 5-year-olds (N = 40) with a repeated chicken game using a novel methodology: Two children each steered an automated toy train carrying a reward. The trains simultaneously moved toward each other so that in order to avoid a crash-which left both children empty-handed-1 train had to swerve. By swerving, however, the trains lost a portion of the rewards so that it was in each child's interest to go straight. Children coordinated their decisions successfully over multiple rounds, and they mostly did so by taking turns at swerving. In dyads in which turn-taking was rare, dominant children obtained significantly higher payoffs than their partners. Moreover, the coordination process was more efficient in turn-taking dyads as indicated by a significant reduction in conflicts and verbal protest. These findings indicate that already by the late preschool years children can independently coordinate decisions with peers in recurrent conflicts of interest.Item Open Access Children use salience to solve coordination problems(Developmental Science, 2015-05) Grueneisen, Sebastian; Wyman, Emily; Tomasello, MichaelItem Open Access Children's developing metaethical judgments.(J Exp Child Psychol, 2017-08-17) Schmidt, Marco FH; Gonzalez-Cabrera, Ivan; Tomasello, MichaelHuman adults incline toward moral objectivism but may approach things more relativistically if different cultures are involved. In this study, 4-, 6-, and 9-year-old children (N=136) witnessed two parties who disagreed about moral matters: a normative judge (e.g., judging that it is wrong to do X) and an antinormative judge (e.g., judging that it is okay to do X). We assessed children's metaethical judgment, that is, whether they judged that only one party (objectivism) or both parties (relativism) could be right. We found that 9-year-olds, but not younger children, were more likely to judge that both parties could be right when a normative ingroup judge disagreed with an antinormative extraterrestrial judge (with different preferences and background) than when the antinormative judge was another ingroup individual. This effect was not found in a comparison case where parties disagreed about the possibility of different physical laws. These findings suggest that although young children often exhibit moral objectivism, by early school age they begin to temper their objectivism with culturally relative metaethical judgments.Item Open Access Children's Intrinsic Motivation to Provide Help Themselves After Accidentally Harming Others.(Child Dev, 2016-11-01) Hepach, Robert; Vaish, Amrisha; Tomasello, MichaelLittle is known about the flexibility of children's prosocial motivation. Here, 2- and 3-year-old children's (n = 128) internal arousal, as measured via changes in pupil dilation, was increased after they accidentally harmed a victim but were unable to repair the harm. If they were able to repair (or if they themselves did not cause the harm and the help was provided by someone else) their arousal subsided. This suggests that children are especially motivated to help those whom they have harmed, perhaps out of a sense of guilt and a desire to reconcile with them. Young children care not only about the well-being of others but also about the relationship they have with those who depend on their help.Item Open Access Children's meta-talk in their collaborative decision making with peers.(J Exp Child Psychol, 2018-02) Köymen, Bahar; Tomasello, MichaelIn collaborative decision making, children must evaluate the evidence behind their respective claims and the rationality of their respective proposals with their partners. In the main study, 5- and 7-year-old peer dyads (N = 196) were presented with a novel animal. In the key condition, children in a dyad individually received conflicting information about what the animal needs (e.g., rocks vs. sand for food) from sources that differ in reliability (with first-hand vs. indirect evidence). Dyads in both age groups were able to reliably settle on the option with the best supporting evidence. Moreover, in making their decision, children, especially 7-year-olds, engaged in various kinds of meta-talk about the evidence and its validity. In a modified version of the key condition in Study 2, 3- and 5-year-olds (N = 120) interacted with a puppet who tried to convince children to change their minds by producing meta-talk. When the puppet insisted and produced meta-talk, 5-year-olds, but not 3-year-olds, were more likely to change their minds if their information was unreliable. These results suggest that even preschoolers can engage in collaborative reasoning successfully, but the ability to reflect on the process by stepping back to jointly examine the evidence emerges only during the early school years.Item Open Access Children's reasoning with peers in cooperative and competitive contexts.(Br J Dev Psychol, 2018-03) Domberg, Andreas; Köymen, Bahar; Tomasello, MichaelWe report two studies that demonstrate how five- and seven-year-olds adapt their production of arguments to either a cooperative or a competitive context. Two games elicited agreements from peer dyads about placing animals on either of two halves of a playing field owned by either child. Children had to produce arguments to justify these decisions. Played in a competitive context that encouraged placing animals on one's own half, children's arguments showed a bias that was the result of withholding known arguments. In a cooperative context, children produced not only more arguments, but also more 'two-sided' arguments. Also, seven-year-olds demonstrated a more frequent and strategic use of arguments that specifically refuted decisions that would favour their peers. The results suggest that cooperative contexts provide a more motivating context for children to produce arguments. Statement of contribution What is already known on this subject? Reasoning is a social skill that allows people to reach joint decisions. Preschoolers give reasons for their proposals in their peer conversations. By adolescence, children use sophisticated arguments (e.g., refutations and rebuttals). What the present study adds? Cooperation offers a more motivating context for children's argument production. Seven-year-olds are more strategic than five-year-olds in their reasoning with peers. Children's reasoning with others becomes more sophisticated after preschool years.Item Open Access Children, chimpanzees, and bonobos adjust the visibility of their actions for cooperators and competitors.(Sci Rep, 2017-08-17) Grueneisen, Sebastian; Duguid, Shona; Saur, Heiko; Tomasello, MichaelChimpanzees and bonobos are highly capable of tracking other's mental states. It has been proposed, however, that in contrast to humans, chimpanzees are only able to do this in competitive interactions but this has rarely been directly tested. Here, pairs of chimpanzees or bonobos (Study 1) and 4-year-old children (Study 2) were presented with two almost identical tasks differing only regarding the social context. In the cooperation condition, players' interests were matched: they had to make corresponding choices to be mutually rewarded. To facilitate coordination, subjects should thus make their actions visible to their partner whose view was partially occluded. In the competition condition, players' interests were directly opposed: the partner tried to match the subject's choice but subjects were only rewarded if they chose differently, so that they benefited from hiding their actions. The apes successfully adapted their decisions to the social context and their performance was markedly better in the cooperation condition. Children also distinguished between the two contexts, but somewhat surprisingly, performed better in the competitive condition. These findings demonstrate experimentally that chimpanzees and bonobos can take into account what others can see in cooperative interactions. Their social-cognitive skills are thus more flexible than previously assumed.Item Open Access Chimpanzees return favors at a personal cost.(Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 2017-06-19) Schmelz, Martin; Grueneisen, Sebastian; Kabalak, Alihan; Jost, Jürgen; Tomasello, MichaelHumans regularly provide others with resources at a personal cost to themselves. Chimpanzees engage in some cooperative behaviors in the wild as well, but their motivational underpinnings are unclear. In three experiments, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) always chose between an option delivering food both to themselves and a partner and one delivering food only to themselves. In one condition, a conspecific partner had just previously taken a personal risk to make this choice available. In another condition, no assistance from the partner preceded the subject's decision. Chimpanzees made significantly more prosocial choices after receiving their partner's assistance than when no assistance was given (experiment 1) and, crucially, this was the case even when choosing the prosocial option was materially costly for the subject (experiment 2). Moreover, subjects appeared sensitive to the risk of their partner's assistance and chose prosocially more often when their partner risked losing food by helping (experiment 3). These findings demonstrate experimentally that chimpanzees are willing to incur a material cost to deliver rewards to a conspecific, but only if that conspecific previously assisted them, and particularly when this assistance was risky. Some key motivations involved in human cooperation thus may have deeper phylogenetic roots than previously suspected.Item Open Access Concern for Group Reputation Increases Prosociality in Young Children.(Psychol Sci, 2018-02) Engelmann, Jan M; Herrmann, Esther; Tomasello, MichaelThe motivation to build and maintain a positive personal reputation promotes prosocial behavior. But individuals also identify with their groups, and so it is possible that the desire to maintain or enhance group reputation may have similar effects. Here, we show that 5-year-old children actively invest in the reputation of their group by acting more generously when their group's reputation is at stake. Children shared significantly more resources with fictitious other children not only when their individual donations were public rather than private but also when their group's donations (effacing individual donations) were public rather than private. These results provide the first experimental evidence that concern for group reputation can lead to higher levels of prosociality.Item Open Access Connecting through Shared Cognition: Social Consequences and Psychological Underpinnings of Sharing Experiences with Others.(2021) Wolf, WouterTo create social closeness, humans engage in a variety of social activities centered around shared experiences, which, remarkably, do not seem to have a non-human equivalent. Recent work with human adults has suggested that one potential key mechanism through which humans connect to others during shared experiences is shared cognition, the capacity to infer shared mental states, with a particular emphasis on joint attention. To better understand the role of shared experiences, and its underlying social cognition, in human social life, we present a series of studies aimed at examining the ontogeny, phylogeny, contextual flexibility, and (social) consequences of sharing experiences with others through joint attention. Chapter 1 establishes the importance of the current empirical work by discussing the relationship between shared cognition and social bonding from an evolutionary perspective and describes the current state of the field. It then discusses several fundamental questions that remain unanswered, which form the core of the current dissertation. In Chapters 2-4, we describe a set of studies aimed at better understanding the ontogeny and phylogeny of the role of joint attention in connecting with others through shared experiences. We find that although both human children and great apes behave more socially after co-attending to a video (Chapters 2 and 3), only humans seem to create additional social closeness by creating common ground with their partner about the fact that they are sharing an experience through mutual gaze in response to a stimulus onset (Chapter 4). In Chapter 5, we describe a study with college students, in which we examine whether the social bonding effect of joint attention also operates in the context of online video mediated interactions, and if this phenomenon is moderated by group size. We find no difference between the joint attention and disjoint attention condition for dyads or groups, suggesting that, regardless of group size, joint attention is not an effective way to create social closeness in video mediated social interactions. In Chapter 6, we describe a study in which we examine whether children have a social preference for experiencing an activity together, through joint attention, versus alone, and whether sharing this experience shapes children’s attitudes towards the content of that experience in general. Our preliminary data (halted due to Covid 19 safety regulations) show no difference in children’s willingness to stay engaged in a video depending on whether they shared the experience of watching that video or watched the video by themselves (social preference). We did, however, find a trend towards children staying engaged longer during the shared experience. Additionally, we found no effect of joint attention to a toy on children’s motivation to play with that toy during subsequent individual exposure (attitude formation). In Chapter 7, we examine the darker side of the role of shared cognition in social bonding, namely how, after a shared experience, we are concerned about making a good impression on others. Specifically, we examine the development of the Liking Gap: the tendency of individuals to, after a brief interaction with a partner, think that their partner evaluated them more negatively than they evaluated their partner. Our results with children between age 4 and 11 show a Liking Gap emerging at age 5, and intensifying between age 5 and 11. Finally, in Chapter 8 we summarize and synthesize the empirical findings, discuss their theoretical contribution and practical implications, and propose avenues for future research. Overall, these studies demonstrate the crucial role of humans’ sophisticated social cognitive abilities in our social life, enabling us to connect with others effectively through shared experiences. However, our results also suggest that the extent to which this social cognitive mechanism operates outside of the context of face to face interactions might be limited. Finally, the current work highlights that these new opportunities for social bonding might also come with new opportunities to worry about the impression we make on others, even at an early age.
Item Restricted Differences in the cognitive skills of bonobos and chimpanzees.(PLoS One, 2010-08-27) Herrmann, Esther; Hare, Brian; Call, Josep; Tomasello, MichaelWhile bonobos and chimpanzees are both genetically and behaviorally very similar, they also differ in significant ways. Bonobos are more cautious and socially tolerant while chimpanzees are more dependent on extractive foraging, which requires tools. The similarities suggest the two species should be cognitively similar while the behavioral differences predict where the two species should differ cognitively. We compared both species on a wide range of cognitive problems testing their understanding of the physical and social world. Bonobos were more skilled at solving tasks related to theory of mind or an understanding of social causality, while chimpanzees were more skilled at tasks requiring the use of tools and an understanding of physical causality. These species differences support the role of ecological and socio-ecological pressures in shaping cognitive skills over relatively short periods of evolutionary time.Item Open Access Differences in the early cognitive development of children and great apes.(Dev Psychobiol, 2014-04) Wobber, Victoria; Herrmann, Esther; Hare, Brian; Wrangham, Richard; Tomasello, MichaelThere is very little research comparing great ape and human cognition developmentally. In the current studies we compared a cross-sectional sample of 2- to 4-year-old human children (n=48) with a large sample of chimpanzees and bonobos in the same age range (n=42, hereafter: apes) on a broad array of cognitive tasks. We then followed a group of juvenile apes (n=44) longitudinally over 3 years to track their cognitive development in greater detail. In skills of physical cognition (space, causality, quantities), children and apes performed comparably at 2 years of age, but by 4 years of age children were more advanced (whereas apes stayed at their 2-year-old performance levels). In skills of social cognition (communication, social learning, theory of mind), children out-performed apes already at 2 years, and increased this difference even more by 4 years. Patterns of development differed more between children and apes in the social domain than the physical domain, with support for these patterns present in both the cross-sectional and longitudinal ape data sets. These results indicate key differences in the pattern and pace of cognitive development between humans and other apes, particularly in the early emergence of specific social cognitive capacities in humans.Item Open Access Direct and indirect reputation formation in nonhuman great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus) and human children (Homo sapiens).(J Comp Psychol, 2013-02) Herrmann, Esther; Keupp, Stefanie; Hare, Brian; Vaish, Amrisha; Tomasello, MichaelHumans make decisions about when and with whom to cooperate based on their reputations. People either learn about others by direct interaction or by observing third-party interactions or gossip. An important question is whether other animal species, especially our closest living relatives, the nonhuman great apes, also form reputations of others. In Study 1, chimpanzees, bonobos, orangutans, and 2.5-year-old human children experienced a nice experimenter who tried to give food/toys to the subject and a mean experimenter who interrupted the food/toy giving. In studies 2 and 3, nonhuman great apes and human children could only passively observe a similar interaction, in which a nice experimenter and a mean experimenter interacted with a third party. Orangutans and 2.5-year-old human children preferred to approach the nice experimenter rather than the mean one after having directly experienced their respective behaviors. Orangutans, chimpanzees, and 2.5-year-old human children also took into account experimenter actions toward third parties in forming reputations. These studies show that the human ability to form direct and indirect reputation judgment is already present in young children and shared with at least some of the other great apes.Item Open Access Effects of “We”-Framing and Prior Discourse on Young Children’s Referential Informativeness(2024) Vasil, JaredSuccessful reference relies on being appropriately informative for listeners. What factors influence informativeness? For example, what factors influence the decision to refer to a ball with the less informative phrase it, as opposed to the more informative phrase the red ball? The present dissertation proposes and investigates a common ground hypothesis and a motivation hypothesis as candidate explanations of young children’s informativeness. Chapter 1 reviews some history of early research pertinent to these hypotheses. Chapters 2 and 3 provide extensive review of recent research pertinent to either hypothesis. Chapter 4 sketches a pragmatistic account of applied statistical inference for investigations of referential informativeness. This account eschews the pseudo-objectivity of traditional statistical practice in favor of a subjectivist Bayesian approach. Chapter 5 puts this way of thinking to work as a tool to compare the relative merits of the common ground and motivation hypotheses. A study is reported that investigated the effects of “we”-framing and prior discourse on 4-year-olds’ informativeness. Four-year-olds learned a novel game from an experimenter, E1. E1 framed the game as conventionally shared or idiosyncratically invented. Then, participants played the game with a new experimenter, E2. E2 framed their play with we”-framing or “you”-framing. Subsequently, participants’ informativeness was recorded when they referred to items in the game for E2. Surprisingly, participants were more informative following conventional compared to idiosyncratic discourse. Less reliably, but equally consistently, this same pattern attended “we”-framing, compared to “you”-framing. In addition, exploratory analyses suggested that participants more often taught E2 with normative language, rather than instrumental language, following idiosyncratic discourse when it was supplemented with “you”-framing. These results suggest that conventional discourse and “we”-framing stoke children’s cooperative motivations and, thereby, their informativeness.
Item Open Access Fair Is Not Fair Everywhere(Psychological Science, 2015-08) Schäfer, Marie; Haun, Daniel BM; Tomasello, MichaelItem Open Access From imitation to implementation: How two- and three-year-old children learn to enforce social norms(British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2017-06) Hardecker, Susanne; Tomasello, MichaelItem Open Access Giving Is Nicer than Taking: Preschoolers Reciprocate Based on the Social Intentions of the Distributor(PLOS ONE, 2016-01-25) Vogelsang, Martina; Tomasello, MichaelItem Open Access Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs.(Science, 2016-10-07) Krupenye, Christopher; Kano, Fumihiro; Hirata, Satoshi; Call, Josep; Tomasello, MichaelHumans operate with a "theory of mind" with which they are able to understand that others' actions are driven not by reality but by beliefs about reality, even when those beliefs are false. Although great apes share with humans many social-cognitive skills, they have repeatedly failed experimental tests of such false-belief understanding. We use an anticipatory looking test (originally developed for human infants) to show that three species of great apes reliably look in anticipation of an agent acting on a location where he falsely believes an object to be, even though the apes themselves know that the object is no longer there. Our results suggest that great apes also operate, at least on an implicit level, with an understanding of false beliefs.
- «
- 1 (current)
- 2
- 3
- »